How We Learn: Why Brains Learn Better Than Any Machine...for Now

4.7 4.7 out of 5 stars | 637 ratings

Price: 15.75

Last update: 02-26-2025


About this item

“There are words that are so familiar they obscure rather than illuminate the thing they mean, and ‘learning’ is such a word. It seems so ordinary, everyone does it. Actually it’s more of a black box, which Dehaene cracks open to reveal the awesome secrets within.” (The New York Times Book Review)

An illuminating dive into the latest science on our brain's remarkable learning abilities and the potential of the machines we program to imitate them.

The human brain is an extraordinary learning machine. Its ability to reprogram itself is unparalleled, and it remains the best source of inspiration for recent developments in artificial intelligence. But how do we learn? What innate biological foundations underlie our ability to acquire new information, and what principles modulate their efficiency?

In How We Learn, Stanislas Dehaene finds the boundary of computer science, neurobiology, and cognitive psychology to explain how learning really works and how to make the best use of the brain’s learning algorithms in our schools and universities, as well as in everyday life and at any age.


Top reviews from the United States

  • C.C.
    5.0 out of 5 stars On the Cutting Edge for Humanity's Good
    Reviewed in the United States on January 29, 2020
    “Just as education is based on biology, the field of education must be grounded in a systematic and rigorous research ecosystem that brings together teachers, patients, and researchers, in a ceaseless search for more effective, evidence-based learning strategies” (245) (from How We Learn).

    How We Learn is Stanislas Dehaene’s fourth book that I have read, and it does not disappoint. Dehaene effortlessly and compassionately moves between the abstract and the useful, carefully and methodically guiding the reader through a veritable mountain range of information from fields as different as neuroscience and education. And The Wall Street Journal got it right for this book as well when it declared (of Reading In The Brain) that Dehaene “never oversimplifies; he takes the time to tell the whole story; and he tells it in a literate way.”

    All in all this is an incredible book, whether you’re interested in neuroscience, education, how brain plasticity and literacy are related, AI or even the brains of babies. There’s really something in it for everyone, whether you’re looking to apply your knowledge to study (or help someone else study) more effectively, or improve your own understanding of how the brain works. Dehaene is on the cutting edge, and he’s incredibly compassionate without ever being tendentious or moralistic. Below is a more detailed breakdown.

    How We Learn is divided into three parts. Part One answers the question “What is Learning?” In the first chapter he discusses seven definitions of learning. One of the most interesting definitions (which isn’t even included among the first seven) is “Learning is inferring the grammar of a domain” in which he submits: “Characteristic of the human species is a relentless search for abstract rules, high-level conclusions that are extracted from a specific situation and subsequently tested on new observations” (35).

    In Chapter 2 Dehaene wrestles for 20 pages with “Why our brain learns better than current machines,” continuing the discussion of learning all the while. Dehaene emphatically disagrees with the belief that “machines are about to overtake us” (27). A handful of the things he argues humans still do much better includes: Learning Abstract concepts; Data-efficient learning; Social learning; One-trial Learning; and, Systematicity and the language of thought.

    in Part 2 Dehaene delves into “How Our Brain Learns.” This is the most scientifically granular section and, for many more technical readers, may be the most interesting. The neuroscience underpinning the four chapters in Part 2 is where Dehaene really shows off how dynamic a mind he has. Essentially, human thought is itself a kind of symbolic language. Furthermore, the literacy of thought starts almost as soon as a baby starts to develop as a fetus. By the time a baby is born, it is an incredibly well-developed instrument ready for its second (rather than first) phase of life, for which it has been preparing for three seasons. Dehaene’s thoughts and work on infants alone in this book is well worth ten times its price.

    Part Three, more of the applied education section, starts with the “Four Pillars of Learning”: Attention (Ch 7, about 30 pages), Active Engagement (Ch 8, about 20 pages), Error Feedback (Ch 9, about 20 pages), Consolidation (Ch 10, about 15 pages). Each of these chapters is a combinatory mine of research, experimental data and studies, as well as practical advice for learners and teachers, reminiscent of Brown, Roediger and McDaniel’s excellent book Make It Stick.

    The following are some kernels of very useful information from Chapters 7-10:

    “The intellectual quotient [IQ] is just a behavioral ability, and as such, it is far from being unchangeable by education. Like any of our abilities, IQ rests on specific brain circuits whose synaptic weights can be changed by training” (167).

    “A passive organism does not learn” (178).

    “To learn, our brain must first form a hypothetical mental model [algorithm] of the outside world, which it then projects onto its environment and puts to a test by comparing its predictions to what it receives from the senses. This algorithm implies an active, engaged, and attentive posture. Motivation is essential: we learn well only if we have a clear goal and we fully commit to reaching it” (178).

    “While it is crucial for students to be motivated, active, and engaged, this does not mean they should be left to their own devices” (184).

    “Pure discovery learning, the idea that children can teach themselves, is one of the many educational myths that have been debunked but still remain curiously popular. […] Two other major misconceptions are linked to it: the myth of the digital native [and] the myth of learning styles” 185).

    “Zero error, zero learning,” but… “We do not need an actual error in order to learn—all we need is an internal sign that travels in the brain” (204)

    “It would be wrong, therefore, to believe that what matters most for learning is to make a lot of mistakes […] What matters is receiving explicit feedback that reduces the learner’s uncertainty. […] The theory of error backpropogation predicts: every unexpected event leads to corresponding adjustment of the internal model of the world" (205).

    “This is the golden rule: it is always better to spread out the training periods rather than cram them into a single run. […] Decades of psychological research show that if you have a fixed amount of time to learn something, spacing out the lessons is a much more effective strategy than grouping them” (218).

    “Sleep and leaning are strongly linked” (228).

    “Computer scientists have already designed several learning algorithms that mimic the sleep/wake cycle” (231).

    “From an educational perspective there is little doubt that improving the length and quality of sleep can be an effective intervention for all children, especially those with learning difficulties” (235).

    Part Three ends with the Dehaene’s “Conclusion: Reconciling Education with Neuroscience.” He conveniently provides a bullet point summary as well as “Thirteen Take-Home Messages to Optimize Children’s Potential.” Here they are, without their supporting paragraphs.

    Do not underestimate children.

    Take advantage of the brain’s sensitivity periods.

    Enrich the environment.

    Rescind the idea that all children are different.

    Pat attention to attention.

    Keep children active, curious, engaged, and autonomous.

    Make every school day enjoyable.

    Encourage efforts.

    Help students deepen their thinking.

    Set clear learning objectives.

    Accept and correct mistakes.

    Practice regularly.

    Let students sleep.

    Dehaene ends with his insistence that “schools should devote more time to parents training,” and that “scientists must engage with teachers and schools in order to consolidate the growing field of educational science” (244).
    Customer image
    C.C.
    5.0 out of 5 stars
    On the Cutting Edge for Humanity's Good

    Reviewed in the United States on January 29, 2020
    “Just as education is based on biology, the field of education must be grounded in a systematic and rigorous research ecosystem that brings together teachers, patients, and researchers, in a ceaseless search for more effective, evidence-based learning strategies” (245) (from How We Learn).

    How We Learn is Stanislas Dehaene’s fourth book that I have read, and it does not disappoint. Dehaene effortlessly and compassionately moves between the abstract and the useful, carefully and methodically guiding the reader through a veritable mountain range of information from fields as different as neuroscience and education. And The Wall Street Journal got it right for this book as well when it declared (of Reading In The Brain) that Dehaene “never oversimplifies; he takes the time to tell the whole story; and he tells it in a literate way.”

    All in all this is an incredible book, whether you’re interested in neuroscience, education, how brain plasticity and literacy are related, AI or even the brains of babies. There’s really something in it for everyone, whether you’re looking to apply your knowledge to study (or help someone else study) more effectively, or improve your own understanding of how the brain works. Dehaene is on the cutting edge, and he’s incredibly compassionate without ever being tendentious or moralistic. Below is a more detailed breakdown.

    How We Learn is divided into three parts. Part One answers the question “What is Learning?” In the first chapter he discusses seven definitions of learning. One of the most interesting definitions (which isn’t even included among the first seven) is “Learning is inferring the grammar of a domain” in which he submits: “Characteristic of the human species is a relentless search for abstract rules, high-level conclusions that are extracted from a specific situation and subsequently tested on new observations” (35).

    In Chapter 2 Dehaene wrestles for 20 pages with “Why our brain learns better than current machines,” continuing the discussion of learning all the while. Dehaene emphatically disagrees with the belief that “machines are about to overtake us” (27). A handful of the things he argues humans still do much better includes: Learning Abstract concepts; Data-efficient learning; Social learning; One-trial Learning; and, Systematicity and the language of thought.

    in Part 2 Dehaene delves into “How Our Brain Learns.” This is the most scientifically granular section and, for many more technical readers, may be the most interesting. The neuroscience underpinning the four chapters in Part 2 is where Dehaene really shows off how dynamic a mind he has. Essentially, human thought is itself a kind of symbolic language. Furthermore, the literacy of thought starts almost as soon as a baby starts to develop as a fetus. By the time a baby is born, it is an incredibly well-developed instrument ready for its second (rather than first) phase of life, for which it has been preparing for three seasons. Dehaene’s thoughts and work on infants alone in this book is well worth ten times its price.

    Part Three, more of the applied education section, starts with the “Four Pillars of Learning”: Attention (Ch 7, about 30 pages), Active Engagement (Ch 8, about 20 pages), Error Feedback (Ch 9, about 20 pages), Consolidation (Ch 10, about 15 pages). Each of these chapters is a combinatory mine of research, experimental data and studies, as well as practical advice for learners and teachers, reminiscent of Brown, Roediger and McDaniel’s excellent book Make It Stick.

    The following are some kernels of very useful information from Chapters 7-10:

    “The intellectual quotient [IQ] is just a behavioral ability, and as such, it is far from being unchangeable by education. Like any of our abilities, IQ rests on specific brain circuits whose synaptic weights can be changed by training” (167).

    “A passive organism does not learn” (178).

    “To learn, our brain must first form a hypothetical mental model [algorithm] of the outside world, which it then projects onto its environment and puts to a test by comparing its predictions to what it receives from the senses. This algorithm implies an active, engaged, and attentive posture. Motivation is essential: we learn well only if we have a clear goal and we fully commit to reaching it” (178).

    “While it is crucial for students to be motivated, active, and engaged, this does not mean they should be left to their own devices” (184).

    “Pure discovery learning, the idea that children can teach themselves, is one of the many educational myths that have been debunked but still remain curiously popular. […] Two other major misconceptions are linked to it: the myth of the digital native [and] the myth of learning styles” 185).

    “Zero error, zero learning,” but… “We do not need an actual error in order to learn—all we need is an internal sign that travels in the brain” (204)

    “It would be wrong, therefore, to believe that what matters most for learning is to make a lot of mistakes […] What matters is receiving explicit feedback that reduces the learner’s uncertainty. […] The theory of error backpropogation predicts: every unexpected event leads to corresponding adjustment of the internal model of the world" (205).

    “This is the golden rule: it is always better to spread out the training periods rather than cram them into a single run. […] Decades of psychological research show that if you have a fixed amount of time to learn something, spacing out the lessons is a much more effective strategy than grouping them” (218).

    “Sleep and leaning are strongly linked” (228).

    “Computer scientists have already designed several learning algorithms that mimic the sleep/wake cycle” (231).

    “From an educational perspective there is little doubt that improving the length and quality of sleep can be an effective intervention for all children, especially those with learning difficulties” (235).

    Part Three ends with the Dehaene’s “Conclusion: Reconciling Education with Neuroscience.” He conveniently provides a bullet point summary as well as “Thirteen Take-Home Messages to Optimize Children’s Potential.” Here they are, without their supporting paragraphs.

    Do not underestimate children.

    Take advantage of the brain’s sensitivity periods.

    Enrich the environment.

    Rescind the idea that all children are different.

    Pat attention to attention.

    Keep children active, curious, engaged, and autonomous.

    Make every school day enjoyable.

    Encourage efforts.

    Help students deepen their thinking.

    Set clear learning objectives.

    Accept and correct mistakes.

    Practice regularly.

    Let students sleep.

    Dehaene ends with his insistence that “schools should devote more time to parents training,” and that “scientists must engage with teachers and schools in order to consolidate the growing field of educational science” (244).
    Images in this review
  • Joseph Psotka
    5.0 out of 5 stars A terrific view of Human learning sadly outdated and in need of a rewrite.
    Reviewed in the United States on January 22, 2025
    Dehaene makes many splendid insights- e.g. about the need for learning(the project manager) to deal with the complexity of the world, which our DNA (the architect) cannot possibly foresee. The example of C. elegans. is perfect.

    However , many other parameters , such as the spacing of our two eyes , the weight and length of our limbs , or the pitch of our voice , all vary , and this is why our brain must adapt to them .

    Also, our science has added many facts which we could not explain otherwise. We now know that 'objects' in the world are really clouds of mysterious things active inside space. Objects are not the solid things of our senses (or DNA) presupposes, but something else mysterious that conflicts with the DNA's predigested reality. We are only beginning to encounter it in Quantum computing and outer space.. Can space objects be entangled?

    Dehaene provides many excellent points about human learning and he couches his comparisons with AI with a provision that AI in the future may overcome these limitations, but his analyses focus mainly on Lecun's CNNs and not the power of LLMs that have arisen in the last few years and certainly not with the multimodal transformers under current development. I agree that CNNs are the best comparison with cognitive abilities, but it is much too early to make these comparisons. Wait another 10 years, and who knows how close we will be to AGI?

    His use of MRIs in young children is fabulous because this is where most unconscious learning develops. He is aware of the power of unconscious learning (which is what AI does so well) but does not actually state that this is the knowledge that is essential for all the learning in schools that follows. His main points about how to improve education are therefore a little misplaced and needs to be updated. I wonder how he would rewrite this book now, because it desperately needs a rewrite since 2018.
  • Bill
    5.0 out of 5 stars Excellent - highly recommended
    Reviewed in the United States on March 6, 2020
    Stanislas DeHaene is an excellent writer. I assume his native language is French, but he writes English much better than most natives. I have read numerous popular books on neuroscience including two other books by DeHaene, one on reading and a more recent one on consciousness. They are all excellent, but this one may be my favorite because he gives some actionable advice for teachers and students. According to DeHaene there are 4 pillars of learning. (1) Attention - on this - he is an expert - our brain does lots of stuff to which we pay no attention but he says learning requires conscious attention. (2) Active engagement - Passive learning is not very efficient - you must get your students actively involved. (Exercises, problem sets, discussion, etc.) (3) Error Correction - according to DeHaene the brain is constantly refining and updating its representation of the outside world by comparing this information with information coming in from outside. It attempts to adjust for any differences and reduce the "error". This suggests lots of tests with immediate feedback. (4) consolidation - both over the short term - sleep is important - and over the longer term - students need to be tested over longer periods and forced to recall what they were supposed to have learned a week, month or year ago. If this all sounds like "common sense" - it may be - but I find it credible coming from an expert. You can find an interview with DeHaene discussing his book at the podcast "Brain Science" with Ginger Campbell.

  • Best Sellers in

     
     

    The Secrets of Character: Writing a Hero Anyone Will Love

    4.6 4.6 out of 5 stars 73
    15.75
     
     

    Nine Nasty Words: English in the Gutter: Then, Now, and Forever

    4.4 4.4 out of 5 stars 1,090
    13.78
     
     

    Summary - Atomic Habits by James Clear: An Easy & Proven Way to Build Good Habits & Break Bad Ones

    5 5 out of 5 stars 1
    6.13
     
     

    Creating Romantic Characters

    4.4 4.4 out of 5 stars 9
    14.95
     
     

    Steal the Show: From Speeches to Job Interviews to Deal-Closing Pitches, How to Guarantee a Standing Ovation for All the Perf

    4.4 4.4 out of 5 stars 720
    17.5
     
     

    Uncommon Sense Teaching: Practical Insights in Brain Science to Help Students Learn

    4.6 4.6 out of 5 stars 483
    15.75
     
     

    Aprender Inglés Completo [Learn Complete English]: De Principiante a Avanzado: Domina el Inglés con Historias Cortas, Frases

    0 0 out of 5 stars 0
    29.95
     
     

    Writing Down the Bones

    4.6 4.6 out of 5 stars 2,388
    31.47